2016-03-04 15:11:05 点击次数:6905 2.00

提高注意力

 

Background

    TDCS has become increasingly popular for applications in clinical and neurocognitive research, with a broad range of effects and effect sizes. TDCS has shown prom-ise for the treatment of depression [1] and stroke [2],and has been documented to produce cognitive en-hancement in healthy subjects in a large number of re-cent studies: TDCS has been shown to facilitate working memory [3], motor learning [4-7], simple somatosensory and visual motion perception learning [8], and memory for word lists [9].
    We have previously found that tDCS can increase learn-ing in a complex visual search task involving detection of target objects hidden in a complex virtual environment that indicate possible threats [10-12]. In these studies, par-ticipants were trained to classify those images as target ob-ject present or target object absent. Participants received anodal tDCS at up to 2.0 mA using 11 cm 2 electrodes dur-ing training with the electrode positioned over 10–10 EEG position F10 (over the right sphenoid bone above inferior frontal cortex). Though no differences were present at baseline in these studies, large improvements in perform-ance occurred for participants receiving 2.0 mA tDCS.Similar to results presented by Iyer et al. [13], these effects were dose-dependent, with performance improvement showing a highly linear relationship with current strength.
    It is unclear from our previous studies what, specific-ally, mediates the effects of tDCS on learning and per-formance of this task. We have found that a combined measure of glutamate and glutamine (Glx), and also N-acetyl aspartate (NAA) are all increased by tDCS under the anodal electrode, but not in the opposite hemisphere[14], which may result in increased neural activity and plasticity, resulting in greater learning. However, another possibility is that tDCS alters functioning in attentional/perceptual networks in such a way as to enhance the perception of targets, leading to greater learning by virtue of greater perceptual acuity. The specific atten-tional processes that may be involved in this affect are uncertain.
    Here we collected three measures of attention using the Attention Networks Test (ANT) [15] in effort to identify the extent to which attention is modified by tDCS over the right inferior frontal cortex. These data were collected along with performance measures on the target learning task reported previously by our lab [11].The ANT is a combination of the cued reaction time (RT) task [16] and the flanker task [17], and it requires participants to indicate with a speeded button press re-sponse whether a centrally presented arrow points to the left or to the right. Efficiency of three attention networks are assessed by measuring how RT is influenced by alert-ing cues (assessing the alerting network), spatial cues(assessing the orienting network), and flankers (assessing the executive network). These three measures have been validated in previous behavioral and imaging studies and represent functionally and anatomically distinct atten-tion networks in the brain [18,19]. Alerting is defined as achieving and maintaining an alert state and has been linked to right hemisphere fronto-parietal brain net-works [20]; orienting measures the efficiency of informa-tion selection from sensory input and is mediated mainly by superior parietal cortices and frontal eye fields[21]; and executive control, a measure of conflict reso-lution among conflicting visual stimuli, is mediated by medial and lateral frontal cortex [22]. We hypothesized that measures of alerting, orienting, and/or executive at-tention would be enhanced with tDCS, and that increased attention in this/these enhanced domain(s) of attention would be positively correlated with object de-tection scores in participants receiving active (2.0 mA)but not sham (0.1 mA) tDCS
 
Results
    There were no significant differences between tDCS groups in age, years of education, gender, ratio of hours slept prior to experimentation to hours normally slept, hours spent per day playing video/computer games, or number of coffee drinkers (all p’s>0.4). TDCS significantly affected partici-pant’s alerting attention (F(1,17)=7.054; p=0.017), with greater alerting reaction time (RT) difference scores for par-ticipants receiving 2.0 mA (49±2 ms; mean±SEM) than those receiving 0.1 mA (31±6 ms; Figure 1). The variance in alerting RT scores for participants receiving 2.0 mA was not significantly different from participants receiving 0.1 mA, as determined by Levene’s test for equality of var-iances (F(17)=2.92; p=0.148), suggesting that there was no significant effect of tDCS on variance of response times.There was no statistically significant effect of tDCS on orienting or executive attention (both p’s>0.1). Further analyses indicated that, for participants receiving 2.0 mA tDCS, alerting scores were significantly correlated with the proportion of hits in the immediate test (r=0.790;p=0.011; Figure 2) and the delayed test (r=0.848;p=0.004; Figure 2), but were not correlated with hits at baseline (before training began). None of these measures were correlated with alerting for participants receiving 0.1 mA tDCS. The proportion of false alarms, d’ , and β were not correlated with alerting in either tDCS group (all p’s>0.1).
    There was a significant difference in reported skin sen-sation during tDCS between tDCS groups for the self-reported measure of heat (t(17)=−2.694; p=0.015), but the difference was small and no participant receiving 0.1 mA tDCS reported heat sensation above the minimum possible score (i.e. 1, out of a 10 point scale), leading to a lack of variance within that group (0.1 mA=1±0;2.0 mA=2.2±1.4). Itching and tingling were not signifi-cantly different between groups and no measure of skin sensation was correlated with any measure of attention(all p’s>0.1). No measure of mood collected at the end of the experiment was significantly different between tDCS groups and mood measures were not correlated with any measure of attention (all p’s>0.1).
 
Discussion
    As we have previously reported, 2.0 mA of anodal tDCS over right inferior frontal cortex significantly increased performance with training of a difficult hidden object detection task.In addition,we found that higher tDCS current was associated with increased measures of alert-ing attention.Furthermore, the proportion of hits was strongly and significantly correlated with attention scores for participants receiving 2.0 mA tDCS. This sug-gests that tDCS enhancement of object detection per-formance is in part related to enhancement of alerting measures of attention.
    One possibility is that enhancement of alerting atten-tion by tDCS led directly to an increased ability to detect objects in the immediate and delayed tests. A heightened state of alertness during testing would be expected to enhance participants’ ability to identify threatening objects hidden within the complex scenes that were used in this task. In this study we targeted the right inferior frontal cortex, a prominent node of the fronto-parietal attention network thought to support alerting attention as measured by the ANT. Enhancement of alerting at-
tention means that, (1) after 2.0 mA tDCS, participants were quicker to respond to the cue stimulus within the 100 ms interval between cue and target onsets, resulting in a faster response to target stimuli, when compared with 0.1 mA controls, or (2) participants receiving 2.0 mA tDCS were able to achieve a heightened state of alertness overall, with greater priming of target percep-tion and/or response. Enhancement of object detection could be explained to some degree by either or both of these interpretations of these results. If participants are faster to achieve a state of cue-induced alertness, then they might have more time to search for threat stimuli in the test images. This is unlikely to have led to the large increases in learning present in this study, however,as participants were given 2 seconds to view the images,and differences in alerting reaction time scores were on the order of 20 ms, or 1/100 of the stimulus duration in the object detection task. Alternatively, a continuously heightened state of alertness could speed processing of stimuli within the complex virtual environment in this task, leading to the ability to process a greater number of objects in the image and/or more time to consider the response to the image before the end of the response window. Another possible explanation for these results is modulation of perception. Perhaps tDCS enhanced perception for both the object detection task and the ANT, leading to greater performance on both measures.If this were the case, however, one might expect that orienting and executive attention would differ between tDCS groups. Further research is needed to disentangle these possible cognitive effects of tDCS over right infer-ior frontal cortex.
    The frontal-parietal attention network assessed by the alerting measure of the ANT has been proposed by Coull et al. [23] to be associated with vigilance in con-tinuous performance tasks and has been specifically implicated in sustained attention during object selection[24]. TDCS may have prolonged sustained attention in this study, leading both to greater hit rate and alerting reaction time scores on the ANT task. Given that the immediate test and ANT were performed after approxi-mately 1.5 hours of experimentation, it is possible that the differences here can be explained in part by differ-ences in the fatigue between the groups receiving 0.1 mA and 2.0 mA tDCS. However, we did not find that the self-reported measure of fatigue assessed by our mood/state questionnaire was related to enhancement of alerting or that it differed between tDCS groups. Future studies might benefit from examining the relationship between tDCS of the right inferior frontal cortex and sustained attention more directly, using other attention tasks aside from the ANT.
    Interestingly, no correlation was found between the alerting attention measure of the ANT and false alarm rate or d’. Our previous studies [11,12] have shown that tDCS significantly reduces false alarm rate and increases d’. It is likely that the results of these previous studies can be explained by tDCS effects on multiple cognitive domains, each of which may account for effects on dif-ferent performance measures for this task. d’ is calcu-lated by taking the difference between normalized hit rate and normalized false alarm rate. When considered here, this suggests that the nonsignificant relationship between alerting and d’ might be explained by a non-significant relationship between alerting and false alarm rate specifically. Perhaps tDCS enhancement of learning and memory during training led to a greater understand-ing of object identities and general rules of object loca-tions, increasing participants’ d’ scores and decreasing false alarms, while enhancement of alerting led to greater achievement and maintenance of visual search performance during the object detection task, but did not improve the false alarm rate. Another possibility is that effects of tDCS on false alarm rate result from increased risk aversion. Fecteau et al.[25] show that an-odal tDCS near the region targeted in this study decreases risky behavior in the Balloon Analog Risk Task(BART), despite incentive for risky behavior. In this con-text, more risk-averse participants might be more cau-tious in responding to ambiguous stimuli. This would lead to a lower proportion of object-present responses in trails where no object was detected by the participant.
    The ANT has previously been used to examine the rela-tionship between attention and various psychiatric and neurological disorders, including borderline personality disorder [26], dyslexia [27], schizophrenia[28], attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder [29], and depression [30].Deficits in alerting as measured by the ANT have been found for elderly individuals relative to a younger popula-tion [31], and alerting scores have been found to vary by subtype of ADHD [32]. Perhaps enhancement of alerting through modulation of the fronto-parietal alerting net-work with tDCS might reduce deficits found in these populations. Research into the application of tDCS to these clinical issues could lead to new treatments and interventions without the necessity of pharmacological therapy.
 
Caveats
    While this study demonstrates compelling evidence of tDCS effects on basic measures of attention, there were several limitations which should be mentioned. We did not collect baseline alerting scores in this study and it is possible that differences demonstrated between partici-pants receiving 0.1 and 2.0 mA tDCS were due to pre-existing differences in alerting. This is unlikely, however,as there was no correlation between alerting scores and measures of performance on the object detection test performed before training began, and there was no dif-ference in object detection at baseline. Also, the design of this experiment was chosen to maximize the effects on object detection performance by placing the anodal electrode over right inferior frontal cortex, but future studies examining the effects of tDCS on measures of at-tention obtained using the ANT might benefit from using multiple electrode configurations, targeting mul-tiple nodes implicated in the different attention net-works proposed by Posner and Peterson [18].
 
Conclusions
    Transcranial direct current stimulation directed at the right inferior frontal cortex led to a significant increase in alerting attention in this study. This result is import-ant both to the neuroscience community, as it demon-strates the role of this brain area in the alerting network,as proposed by Posner and Peterson [18], and clinically,as alerting deficits are characteristic of a various neuro-cognitive disorders. Additionally, this effect was strongly
correlated with our previously reported effects of tDCS on object detection. These results suggest that the effects of tDCS on alerting are related to effects of tDCS on hit rate after training, but not on false alarm rate or d’. Enhancement of alternative cognitive mechanisms,such as increased neuronal plasticity during training or decreased impulsivity in responding during test, may ac-count for the effects of tDCS on false alarm rate and d’.
 
Methods
    Participants
    Twenty healthy participants gave informed consent and participated in this experiment. One subject performed less than two standard deviations below the mean and was excluded from further analysis. Of the remaining 19 participants (11 male, age=23.4 yrs, 7.7 yrs SD), 9 were randomly assigned to receive 2.0 mA tDCS, while 10 received 0.1 mA. All participants met the following in-
clusion criteria: English as a first language, no history of head injuries or concussions resulting in loss of con-sciousness or hospitalization, right-handedness accord-ing to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [33], no history of psychiatric or neurological disorders, alcohol
or drug abuse, or current medication affecting the CNS,and good or corrected vision and hearing. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Uni-versity of New Mexico. The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest in this research.
 
Target object detection task
    This experiment was designed to test subject’s ability to learn to detect hidden and camouflaged objects placed in a complex virtual environment. In order to engage and maintain a typical participant’s interest, it was designed to be similar to modern video games, which
often include a wartime theme. Five-second video clips from training scenarios from the DARWARS virtual reality training environment were captured for use as feedback in the task [34]. Still images were extracted from these videos and edited to include or remove spe-cific objects. Examples of images presented during train-ing and testing and a full description of the threat detection task can be found in Figure 3, as well as Coff-man et al. [11] and Clark et al. [10].
    Participants were first tested for their baseline ability to detect target objects before training, after which parti-cipants were trained to detect target objects while re-ceiving either 0.1 mA or 2.0 mA tDCS for 30 minutes.Participants were tested immediately after training was completed (immediate test) and again one hour after the end of training (delayed test; Figure 4). Baseline, imme-diate, and one-hour delayed tests each consisted of 100 images presented with no feedback, which were different for each phase of testing. Training sessions consisted of four 11-minute blocks of 60 trials, each of which included an image and appropriate audiovisual feedback,with short rest periods between blocks. Each image was presented for two seconds with an inter-trial interval that varied randomly across trials ranging from four to eight seconds. Participants were instructed to scan the images for target objects with no prior information given about the nature of the target objects. Thus, the partici-pant discovered the correct and incorrect responses to each image after examining the audiovisual feedback on each training trial. The one-hour delayed test was designed to examine retention of learned target object detection ability.
 
Attention network task
    Immediately before the one-hour delayed test, partici-pants completed a 20-minute version of the ANT to as-sess participant’s alerting, orienting, and conflict resolution measures of attention (Figure 4). The ANT was performed as described by Fan et al. [15], with the exception of response method. Participants responded using the same two fingers and keypad that was used in the target object detection task rather than using the left and right mouse buttons and thumbs. The ANT requires participants to determine the direction (left or right) of a target arrow that appears directly above or below a cen-tral fixation that may or may not be accompanied by flankers. These flankers, if present, may point the same direction as the target arrow (congruent) or the opposite direction of the target arrow (incongruent). Additionally,the presentation of the target arrow may or may not be preceded by a temporal or spatial cue, consisting of an asterisk (*). Temporal cues occurred 100 ms before the stimulus and were presented either at the fixation point(center cue) or at both possible target locations (double cue). Spatial cues also occurred 100 ms before the stimulus and were presented at the spatial location of the target arrow.
    The alerting effect is calculated by subtracting the mean RT of the double-cue condition from the mean RT of the no-cue condition, as neither of those condi-tions provide information concerning whether the target would appear above or below the fixation point (spatial information). Without a warning cue (the no-cue condi-tion), attention remains diffused across or oscillating be-tween the upper and lower possible target locations; the double-cue condition impacts attention in the same way,except it alerts the participant to the imminent appear-ance of the target. The orienting effect is calculated by subtraction of the mean RT of the spatial-cue condition from the mean RT of the center-cue condition. Both of these conditions provide information concerning the impending presentation of a target, but the spatial-cue carries the additional information of target location,allowing subjects to orient attention to the appropriate location prior to target presentation. The executive con-trol effect (conflict resolution) is calculated by subtract-ing the mean RT of the congruent flanking conditions(all five arrows pointing the same direction) from the mean RT of the incongruent flanking conditions (the target arrow pointing the opposite direction of the flank-ing arrows).
 
Transcranial direct current stimulation
    Anodal tDCS was delivered for 30 minutes near 10–10 EEG location F10, over the right sphenoid bone. The lo-cation near F10 was suggested from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and magnetoencephalography(MEG) studies of changes in brain networks associated with the acquisition of expertise in this task [10]. TDCS was administered through 11 cm 2 square saline-soaked sponge electrodes. The cathode was placed on the sub-ject’s left upper arm. Electrodes were secured to the scalp and upper arm using Coban self-adherent wrap.TDCS was initiated five minutes before training and continued throughout the first two of four training blocks (Figure 4). We chose this timing because the effects of tDCS have been demonstrated to continue after current administration is ended for at least as long as stimulation was administered [35]. Current was set to either 0.1 mA or 2.0 mA. A current strength of 0.1 mA(the lowest setting on our stimulation device) was selected as our control condition in order to induce physical sensation associated with tDCS (e.g. tingling and/or itching) without stimulating the brain areas be-neath. Recent research has shown that traditional meth-ods of “sham“ tDCS stimulation involving ramping the current on and then off after a short (usually 30 seconds)duration may not be as effective in blinding participants to stimulation condition as previously thought [36]. Simulation studies by Miranda et al. [37] suggest that current strengths less than 0.5 mA at the electrode size used in this study have no effect on brain activity in neural tissue 12 mm below the skin surface. In our con-trol condition, participants received less than 20% of this current strength. Additionally, modulation of motor evoked potential amplitude does not seem to occur at current strengths equal to or below 0.2 mA [38], so we are confident that this stimulation condition had little or no effect on the brain.
    Both participants and experimenters were blind to the amount of current delivered. Experimenter blinding was accomplished using a coded switch box, with inputs for positive and negative leads from two current generators and outputs for only two electrodes, one anode and one cathode. One current generator was set to 0.1 mA and the other was set to 2.0 mA. A six-way switch inter-rupted the circuit, with three settings supplying current to the output leads from one current generator, and the remaining three supplying the output from the other current generator. The inputs that were not actively sup-plying current to the output leads were routed through a simple circuit loop to maintain the activity of the in-active current generator. The six-way switch was coded by a third party to ensure experimenter blinding.
    During tDCS, participants were asked to describe their physical sensations at 1, 5, and 20 minutes after the start of tDCS. Subjects were asked to report sensation on three 10-point Likert scales for itching, tingling, and heat. TDCS was stopped if participants reported a seven or higher on any scale, though no subjects reported this level of sensation. As an additional safety measure, and to elucidate possible state-dependent effects of tDCS,mood/state was assessed before and after experimenta-tion using 6-point Likert scales for the following nine measures: (1) I feel nervous or excited. (2) I feel tired or fatigued. (3) I feel confused or disoriented. (4) I feel sad or down. (5) I feel tense or frustrated. (6) I feel dizzy or light-headed. (7) I feel nauseous. (8) Physically, I feel pain or discomfort. (9) I feel unable to concentrate or pay attention.
 
Data analysis
    We first compared participants’ ANT scores for alerting,orienting, and executive attention using a 2 (tDCS con-dition: 0.1 mA vs 2.0 mA) x 3 (alerting vs orienting vs executive attention) ANOVA. Post-hoc comparisons be-tween tDCS conditions were made using individual Stu-dent’s t-tests, with a Bonferroni corrected α=0.017(0.05/3). We then used Pearson correlation to assess the relationship between attention measures identified as significantly affected by tDCS (alerting was the only such measure) and measures of signal detection (d’, β, hits and false alarms) in the target object detection task. Cor-relation coefficients were computed separately for parti-cipants in receiving 0.1 mA or 2.0 mA tDCS. d’ and β were calculated based on the hits (correct responses to images containing target objects) and false alarm (incor-rect responses to images not containing target objects)rates according to calculations described by Stanislaw and Todorov [39]. Briefly, signal detection (d’) is calcu-lated by subtracting the z-normalized false alarm rate from the z-normalized hit rate, while response bias (β) is calculated by raising e to the power of ½ the difference between the squared, z-normalized false alarm rate and the squared, z-normalized hit rate. Student’s t-test was used to compare tDCS-induced sensation and mood/ state between groups and Pearson correlation was used to examine the relationship between sensation, measures of mood/state, and measures of attention identified as significantly affected by tDCS.
 
Competing interests
    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
 
Authors’ contributions
    BC conceived the study, participated in design of the study, assisted with data collection, performed data analysis, and drafted the manuscript. MT participated in design of the study, assisted with data collection, and helped to draft the manuscript. VC participated in design of the study, helped to coordinate the study, managed experimenter blinding procedures, and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
 
Author details
    1 The Department of Psychology,University of New Mexico, MSC03-2220,Albuquerque, NM 87131-1161, USA.
    2 The Department of Neurosciences,University of New Mexico, MSC08-4740, Albuquerque, NM 87131-5223, USA.
    3 Clinical Neuroscience Center, Dept. Psychology, University of New Mexico,MSC03-2220,Albuquerque, NM 87131-1161, USA.
    4 The Mind Research Network and Lovelace Respiratory Research Institute, 1101 Yale NE,Albuquerque, NM 87106, USA.
 
References
    1. Rigonatti SP, Boggio PS, Myczkowski ML, Otta E, Fiquer JT, Ribeiro RB,Nitsche MA, Pascual-Leone A, Fregni F: Transcranial direct stimulation and fluoxetine for the treatment of depression. Eur Psychiat 2008, 23:74–76.
    2. Webster BR, Celnik PA, Cohen LG: Noninvasive brain stimulation in stroke rehabilitation. NeuroRx 2006, 3:474–481.
    3. Fregni F, Boggio PS, Nitsche M, Bermpohl F, Antal A, Feredoes E, Marcolin MA, Rigonatti SP, Silva MTA, Paulus W: Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working memory. Exp Brain Res 2005, 166:23–30.
    4. Antal A, Nitsche MA, Kincses TZ, Kruse W, Hoffmann KP, Paulus W:Facilitation of visuo-motor learning by transcranial direct current stimulation of the motor and extrastriate visual areas in humans.Eur J Neurosci 2004, 19:2888–2892.
    5. Galea JM, Celnik P: Brain polarization enhances the formation and retention of motor memories. J Neurophys 2009, 102:294.
    6. Reis J, Robertson E, Krakauer JW, Rothwell J, Marshall L, Gerloff C,Wassermann E, Pascual-Leone A, Hummel F, Celnik PA, Classen J, Floel A,Ziemann U, Paulus W, Siebner HR, Born J, Cohen LG: Consensus: “Can tDCS and TMS enhance motor learning and memory formation?”. Brain Stimul 2008, 1:363.
    7. Reis J, Schambra HM, Cohen LG, Buch ER, Fritsch B, Zarahn E, Celnik PA,Krakauer JW: Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. P Natl A Sci 2009, 106:1590.
    8. Ragert P, Vandermeeren Y, Camus M, Cohen LG: Improvement of spatial tactile acuity by transcranial direct current stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol 2008, 119:805–811.
    9. Marshall L, Molle M, Hallschmid M, Born J: Transcranial direct current
stimulation during sleep improves declarative memory. J Neurosci 2004,
24:9985.
    10. Clark VP, Coffman BA, Mayer AR, Weisend MP, Lane TD, Calhoun VD,Raybourn EM, Garcia CM, Wassermann EM: TDCS guided using fMRI significantly accelerates learning to identify concealed objects.Neuroimage 2012, 59:117–128.
    11. Coffman BA, Garcia CM, Van der Merwe A, Barrow R, Calhoun VD, Puffer D,Raybourn EM, Wassermann EM, Weisend MP, Clark VP: Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (TDCS) Accelerated Learning of Covert Threat Detection is influenced by Current Strength and Stimulus Novelty.Neuropsychologia, in press.
    12. Falcone B, Coffman BA, Clark VP, Parasuraman R: Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Augments Perceptual Sensitivity and 24-Hour Retention in a Complex Threat Detection Task. PLoS One, . in press.
    13. Iyer MB, Mattu U, Grafman J, Lomarev M, Sato S, Wassermann EM: Safety and cognitive effect of frontal DC brain polarization in healthy individuals. Neurology 2005, 64:872–875.
    14. Clark VP, Coffman BA, Trumbo MC, Gasparovic C: Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) produces localized and specific alterations in neurochemistry: A 1 H magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Neurosci Lett 2011, 500:67–71.
    15. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI: Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci 2002,14:340–347.
    16. Posner MI: Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 1980, 32:3–25.
    17. Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW: Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Atten Percept Psycho 1974, 16:143–149.
    18. Posner MI, Petersen SE: The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Nurosci 1990, 13:25–42.
    19. Fan J, Posner M: Human attentional networks. Psychiat Prax 2004,31:S210–S214.
    20. Coull JT, Nobre AC, Frith CD: The noradrenergic α2 agonist clonidine modulates behavioural and neuroanatomical correlates of human attentional orienting and alerting. Cereb Cortex 2001, 11:73–84.
    21. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI: The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage 2005, 26:471–479.
    22. Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI: Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 2000, 4:215–222.
    23. Coull JT, Frackowiak RSJ, Frith CD: Monitoring for target objects: activation
of right frontal and parietal cortices with increasing time on task.
Neuropsychologia 1998, 36:1325–1334.
    24. Coull JT, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS, Grasby PM: A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: a PET study of sustained attention and working memory. Neuropsychologia 1996, 34:1085–1095.
    25. Facteau S, Pascual-Leone A, Zald DH, Liguori P, Theoret H, Boggio PS, Fregni F: Activation of prefrontal cortex by transcranial direct current stimulation reduces appetite for risk during ambiguous decision making.J Neurosci 2007, 27:6212–6218.
    26. Rogosch FA, Cicchetti D: Child maltreatment, attention networks, and potential precursors to borderline personality disorder. Dev Psychopathol 2005, 17:1071–1089.
    27. Bednarek DB, Saldaria D, Quintero-Gallego F, Garcia I, Grabowska A, Gomez CM: Attentional deficit in dyslexia: A general or specific impairment?Neuroreport 2004, 15:1787–1790.
    28. Wang K, Fan J, Dong Y, Wang C, Lee TMC, Posner MI: Selective impairment of attentional networks of orienting and executive control in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2005, 78:235–241.
    29. Adolfsdottir S, Sorensen L, Lundervold A: The attention network test: A characteristic pattern of deficits in children with ADHD. Behav Brain Funct 2008, 4:9.
    30. Murphy CF, Alexopoulos GS: Attention network dysfunction and treatment response of geriatric depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2006,28:96–100.
    31. Jennings JM, Dagenbach D, Engle CM, Funke LJ: Age-related changes and the attention network task: An examination of alerting, orienting, and executive function. Aging Neuropsychol 2007, 14:353–369.
    32. Booth JE, Carlson CL, Tucker DM: Performance on a neurocognitive measure of alerting differentiates ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes: A preliminary report. Arch Clin Neuropsych 2007, 22:423–432.
    33. Oldfield RC: The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971, 9:97–113.
    34. MacMillan J, Tomlinson R, Alexander AL, Weil SA, Littleton B, Aptima I:DARWARS: An Architecture That Supports Effective Experiential Training.DARWARS Research Papers 2005, http://www. darwars. com/downloads/2005% 20IITSEC% 20White% 20Paper% 20v2. pdf.
    35. Liebetanz D, Nitsche MA, Tergau F, Paulus W: Pharmacological approach to the mechanisms of transcranial DC-stimulation-induced after-effects of human motor cortex excitability. Brain 2002, 125:2238–2247.
    36. Poreisz C, Boros K, Antal A, Paulus W: Safety aspects of transcranial direct current stimulation concerning healthy participants and patients. Brain Res Bulletin 2007, 72:208–214.
    37. Miranda PC, Faria P, Hallett M: What does the ratio of injected current to electrode area tell us about current density in the brain during tDCS?Clin Neurophysiol 2009, 120:1183–1187.
    38. Nitsche MA, Paulus W: Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol 2000,527:633–639.
    39. Stanislaw H, Todorov N: Calculation of signal detection theory measures.Beh Res Meth Instr 1999, 31:137–149.
主评论
8/10
支持者
2/10
文笔质量
4.5/10

7.5

一般
3.50
总结

该文章的阅读量尚可,因为支持者占绝对优势,所以可信度尚可.

我的留言

登陆

最新消息

前沿

Live深度脑部刺激:窃听大脑内的故障

据英国《自然》杂志网站近日报道,研究表明,深度脑部刺激设备已经在治疗帕金森病等常见的神经系统变性疾病 [...]

2016/2/6 22:26:47 16 0